| Peer-Reviewed

On Arabic Abstract and Concrete Words Recall Using Free Recall Paradigms: Is It Abstractness, Concreteness, or Zero Effect

Received: 19 May 2015     Accepted: 3 June 2015     Published: 25 June 2015
Views:       Downloads:
Abstract

Purpose: To see whether abstract or concrete words are better recalled in free recall type and to measure primacy and recency displayed effects in free recall paradigms. Method: 9 undergraduates in King Saud University, Saudi Arabia, participated in this study where they were trained to differentiate between abstract and concrete words. Then, a list of 20 Arabic abstract and concrete words was given to them to be classified into abstract and concrete words based on four factors: concreteness, imageability, meaningfulness, and age of acquisition. An observation sheet was provided to the experiment administrator to document observed recall effects and recalled words. Three methods were used to facilitate this experiment: auditory, visual, and hand-writing. Five of the participants were asked to recall freely only 5 Arabic abstract words and 5 concrete words. On the hand, 4 were asked to to recall freely the 10 Arabic abstract words and 10 Arabic concrete words. Results: Descriptive and referential statistics tools were run to analyse the collected date. The computed referential statistics tools indicated generally acceptable values and positive usability of the administered measures in this study. Descriptive statistics results indicated a (100) score for the frequency of zero effect over both abstractness and concreteness effects. Results also indicated a negative effect of list length of Arabic abstract and concrete words where the total number of words was recalled completely in both short and long lists of words. Finally, recency effect approved an advantage over primacy effect with (65%) for the former and only (35%) for the latter. Conclusions: There was neither an advantage for abstract words over concrete ones nor an advantage for concrete words over the abstract ones, it was a rather a zero effect. List length effect didn’t affect memory recall during free recall paradigms. Last but not the least, recency effect is more frequent than primacy effect in free recall paradigms.

Published in Psychology and Behavioral Sciences (Volume 4, Issue 4)
DOI 10.11648/j.pbs.20150404.13
Page(s) 154-164
Creative Commons

This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited.

Copyright

Copyright © The Author(s), 2015. Published by Science Publishing Group

Keywords

Abstract Words, Concrete Words, Free Recall, Primacy Effect, Recency Effect, List Length Effect, Concreteness Effect, Abstractness Effect, Zero Effect

References
[1] Bauer, L. M., & Altarriba, J. (2008). An Investigation of Sex Differences in Word Ratings across Concrete, Abstract, and Emotion Words. The Psychological Record, 58(3), 465-474. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/212759773?accountid=142908
[2] Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2013). The acquisition of abstract words by young infants. Cognition, 127(3), 391-397. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/1413415110?accountid=142908
[3] Binder, J. R., Westbury, C. F., McKiernan, K. A., Possing, E. T., & Medler, D. A. (2005). Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(6), pp. 905-17.
[4] Borghi, A. M., Flumini, A., Cimatti, F., Marocco, D., & Scorolli, C. (2011). Manipulating objects and telling words: A study on concrete and abstract words acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 15. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00015
[5] Campos, A. (2009). Imagery Vividness and Emotionality of Concrete and Abstract Words: A Classroom Activity. Revista Galego-Portuguesa De Psicoloxía E Educación, 17(1,2), pp. 1138-1663.
[6] Caramelli, N., Setti, A., & Maurizzi, D. D. (2004). Concrete and abstract concepts in school age children. Psychology of Language and Communication, 8(2), pp. 19-34.
[7] Crutch, S. J., & Warrington, E. K. (2005). Abstract and concrete concepts have structurally different representational frameworks. Brain, 128(pt 3), pp. 615-27.
[8] Dahlstrom, D., & Ultis, J. (2014, March 1). Modeling Reaction Time for Abstract and Concrete Concepts using a Recurrent Network. San Diego, San Diego, USA.
[9] Dewey, R. A. (2007). Psychology: An Introduction. Psych Web.
[10] Dukes, W. F., & Bastian, J. (1966). Recall of abstract and concrete words equated for meaningfulness. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5(5), pp. 455–458.
[11] Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A., Afonso, O., Scheepers, C., & Carreiras, M. (2008). Qualitative differences in the representation of abstract versus concrete words: evidence from the visual-world paradigm. Cognition, 110(2), pp. 284-92.
[12] Farley, A. P., Ramonda, K., & Liu, X. (2012). The concreteness effect and the bilingual lexicon: The impact of visual stimuli attachment on meaning recall of abstract L2 words. Language Teaching Research, 16(4), 449-466. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1362168812436910
[13] Faust, M. (Ed.). (2012). The Handbook of the Neuropsychology of Language. West Sussex: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
[14] Fliessbach, K., Weis, S., Klaver, P., Elger, C. E., & Weber, B. (2006). The effect of word concreteness on recognition memory. NeuroImage, 32(3), 1413-1421. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.007
[15] Hanley, J. R., Hunt, R. P., Steed, D. A., & Jackman, S. (2013). Concreteness and word production. Memory & Cognition, 41(3), 365-377. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/1347611949?accountid=142908
[16] Harad, L., & Coch, D. (2009). Remembering abstract vs. concrete words. Poster presented at the 18th Annual Wetterhahn Undergraduate Science Poster Symposium, Women in Science Project (WISP), Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire, USA.
[17] Il'yuchenok, ,I.R., Sysoeva, O. V., & Ivanitskii, A. M. (2008). Two semantic systems in the brain for rapid and slow differentiation of abstract and concrete words. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 38(9), 963-70. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11055-008-9083-5
[18] Lagishetti, S. K., & Goswami, S. P. (2012). Measurement of Reaction Time for Processing of Concrete and Abstract Words. Journal of All India Institute of Speech and Hearing JAIISH, 31, pp. 139-144.
[19] Marques, J. F., & Nunes, L. D. (2012). The contributions of language and experience to the representation of abstract and concrete words: Different weights but similar organizations. Memory & Cognition, 40(8), 1266-1275. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/1266030094?accountid=142908
[20] Mestres-Missé, A., Münte, T. F., & Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2009). Functional neuroanatomy of contextual acquisition of concrete and abstract words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), pp. 2154-71.
[21] MRC-Psycholinguistic-Database-Editors. (1987). MRC Psycholinguistic Database: Dict Utility Interface. Retrieved from MRC Psycholinguistic Database: http://websites.psychology.uwa.edu.au/school/MRCDatabase/uwa_mrc.htm
[22] Nietzsche, F. (2006). Abstract vs.Concrete. In R. B. Parkinson, Writing for Results (pp. 1-13).
[23] Oxford-University-Press-Editors. (2014). Free recall . Retrieved from Oxfordindex.oup.com/: http://oxfordindex.oup.com/search?q=free+recall
[24] Paivio, A. (1965). Abstractness, imagery, and meaningfulness in paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, pp. 32-38.
[25] Paivio, A. (1968). A factor-analytic study of word attributes and verbal learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, pp. 41-49.
[26] Paivio, A. (1974). Spacing of Repetitions in the Incidental and Intentional Free. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, pp. 497-511.
[27] Paivio, A. (1975). Coding distinctions and repetition effects in memory. London: Dept. of Psychology, University of Western Ontario.
[28] Paivio, A. (1990). Mental Representations: A Dual Coding Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[29] Paivio, A., & Csapo, K. (1973). Picture superiority in free recall: Imagery or dual coding? Cognitive Psychology, 5, pp. 176-206.
[30] Paivio, A., & Desrochers, A. (1981). A dual-coding approach to bilingual memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 20, pp. 532-539.
[31] Paivio, A., & Foth, D. (1970). Imaginal and verbal mediators and noun concreteness in paired-associate learning: The elusive interaction. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, pp. 384-390.
[32] Paivio, A., & Okovita, H. W. (1971). Word imagery modalities and associative learning in blind and sighted subjects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, pp. 506-510.
[33] Paivio, A., & Yuille, J. C. (1966). Word abstractness and meaningfulness, and paired-associate learning in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 4(1), 81-89. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/84317893?accountid=142908
[34] Pichette, F., de Serres, L., & Lafontaine, M. (2012). Sentence reading and writing for second language vocabulary acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 33(1), 66-82. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/964182028?accountid=142908
[35] Pobric, G., Lambon Ralph, M.,A., & Jefferies, E. (2009). The role of the anterior temporal lobes in the comprehension of concrete and abstract words: RTMS evidence. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 45(9), 1104-1110. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.02.006
[36] Prior, M. R., Cumming, G., & Hendy, J. (1984). Recognition of abstract and concrete words in a dichotic listening paradigm. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 20(1), 149-157. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/81071377?accountid=142908
[37] Psychology-Dictionary-Team. (2012). Psychology Dictionary Online . Psychology Dictionary.
[38] Psychology-Encyclopedia-Editors. (2014). Psychology Encyclopedia: Free recall . Retrieved from Psychology.jrank.org/: http://psychology.jrank.org/pages/255/Free-Recall-Learning.html
[39] Rastatter, M., Dell, C. W., McGuire, R. A., & Loren, C. (1987). Vocal reaction times to unilaterally presented concrete and abstract words: Towards a theory of differential right hemispheric semantic processing. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 23(1), 135-142. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/77482560?accountid=142908
[40] Salsbury, T., Crossley, S. A., & McNamara, D. S. (2011). Psycholinguistic word information in second language oral discourse. Second Language Research, 27(3), 343-360. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0267658310395851
[41] Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Akin, C. E. (1993). Developmental trends in lexical decisions for abstract and concrete words. reading research report no. 1. (). Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/62860333?accountid=142908
[42] Schwanenflugel, P. J., Akin, C., & Luh, W. (1992). Context availability and the recall of abstract and concrete words. Memory & Cognition (Pre-2011), 20(1), 96-104. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/217453817?accountid=142908
[43] Schwanenflugel, P. J., Henderson, R. L., & Fabricius, W. V. (1998). Developing organization of mental verbs and theory of mind in middle childhood: Evidence from extensions. Developmental Psychology, 34(3), 512-524. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/62451398?accountid=142908
[44] Van-Ness, M. (2014). Recency Effects. Retrieved from Springerreference.com: http://www.springerreference.com/docs/html/chapterdbid/333134.html
[45] Walker, I., & Hulme, C. (1999). Concrete words are easier to recall than abstract words: Evidence for a semantic contribution to short-term serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25(5), 1256-1271. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/214370571?accountid=142908
[46] Wang, J., Conder, J. A., Blitzer, D. N., & Shinkareva, S. V. (2010). Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 31(10), pp. 1459-68.
[47] Weiss, S., Müller, H. M., Wörmann, F., & Mertens, M. (2011). "Tooth and Truth": Brain Activation During Passive Listening to Concrete and Abstract Nouns. The Open Behavioral Science Journal, pp. 37-47.
[48] West, W. C., & Holcomb, P. J. (2000). Imaginal, semantic, and surface-level processing of concrete and abstract words: An electrophysiological investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1024-1037. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/72554202?accountid=142908
[49] Wiemer-Hastings, K., & Xu, X. (2005). Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Science, 29(5), 719-736. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/62087284?accountid=142908
[50] Yao, B., Vasiljevic, M., Weick, M., Sereno, M. E., O'Donnell, P.,J., & Sereno, S. C. (2013). Semantic size of abstract concepts: It gets emotional when you cant see it. PLoS One, 8(9) doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075000.
Cite This Article
  • APA Style

    Nasser Saleh Al-Mansour, Yasir Saad Almukhaizeem, Ahmed Mohammed Alduais. (2015). On Arabic Abstract and Concrete Words Recall Using Free Recall Paradigms: Is It Abstractness, Concreteness, or Zero Effect. Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 4(4), 154-164. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20150404.13

    Copy | Download

    ACS Style

    Nasser Saleh Al-Mansour; Yasir Saad Almukhaizeem; Ahmed Mohammed Alduais. On Arabic Abstract and Concrete Words Recall Using Free Recall Paradigms: Is It Abstractness, Concreteness, or Zero Effect. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2015, 4(4), 154-164. doi: 10.11648/j.pbs.20150404.13

    Copy | Download

    AMA Style

    Nasser Saleh Al-Mansour, Yasir Saad Almukhaizeem, Ahmed Mohammed Alduais. On Arabic Abstract and Concrete Words Recall Using Free Recall Paradigms: Is It Abstractness, Concreteness, or Zero Effect. Psychol Behav Sci. 2015;4(4):154-164. doi: 10.11648/j.pbs.20150404.13

    Copy | Download

  • @article{10.11648/j.pbs.20150404.13,
      author = {Nasser Saleh Al-Mansour and Yasir Saad Almukhaizeem and Ahmed Mohammed Alduais},
      title = {On Arabic Abstract and Concrete Words Recall Using Free Recall Paradigms: Is It Abstractness, Concreteness, or Zero Effect},
      journal = {Psychology and Behavioral Sciences},
      volume = {4},
      number = {4},
      pages = {154-164},
      doi = {10.11648/j.pbs.20150404.13},
      url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20150404.13},
      eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.pbs.20150404.13},
      abstract = {Purpose: To see whether abstract or concrete words are better recalled in free recall type and to measure primacy and recency displayed effects in free recall paradigms. Method: 9 undergraduates in King Saud University, Saudi Arabia, participated in this study where they were trained to differentiate between abstract and concrete words. Then, a list of 20 Arabic abstract and concrete words was given to them to be classified into abstract and concrete words based on four factors: concreteness, imageability, meaningfulness, and age of acquisition. An observation sheet was provided to the experiment administrator to document observed recall effects and recalled words. Three methods were used to facilitate this experiment: auditory, visual, and hand-writing. Five of the participants were asked to recall freely only 5 Arabic abstract words and 5 concrete words. On the hand, 4 were asked to to recall freely the 10 Arabic abstract words and 10 Arabic concrete words. Results: Descriptive and referential statistics tools were run to analyse the collected date. The computed referential statistics tools indicated generally acceptable values and positive usability of the administered measures in this study. Descriptive statistics results indicated a (100) score for the frequency of zero effect over both abstractness and concreteness effects. Results also indicated a negative effect of list length of Arabic abstract and concrete words where the total number of words was recalled completely in both short and long lists of words. Finally, recency effect approved an advantage over primacy effect with (65%) for the former and only (35%) for the latter. Conclusions: There was neither an advantage for abstract words over concrete ones nor an advantage for concrete words over the abstract ones, it was a rather a zero effect. List length effect didn’t affect memory recall during free recall paradigms. Last but not the least, recency effect is more frequent than primacy effect in free recall paradigms.},
     year = {2015}
    }
    

    Copy | Download

  • TY  - JOUR
    T1  - On Arabic Abstract and Concrete Words Recall Using Free Recall Paradigms: Is It Abstractness, Concreteness, or Zero Effect
    AU  - Nasser Saleh Al-Mansour
    AU  - Yasir Saad Almukhaizeem
    AU  - Ahmed Mohammed Alduais
    Y1  - 2015/06/25
    PY  - 2015
    N1  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20150404.13
    DO  - 10.11648/j.pbs.20150404.13
    T2  - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences
    JF  - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences
    JO  - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences
    SP  - 154
    EP  - 164
    PB  - Science Publishing Group
    SN  - 2328-7845
    UR  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20150404.13
    AB  - Purpose: To see whether abstract or concrete words are better recalled in free recall type and to measure primacy and recency displayed effects in free recall paradigms. Method: 9 undergraduates in King Saud University, Saudi Arabia, participated in this study where they were trained to differentiate between abstract and concrete words. Then, a list of 20 Arabic abstract and concrete words was given to them to be classified into abstract and concrete words based on four factors: concreteness, imageability, meaningfulness, and age of acquisition. An observation sheet was provided to the experiment administrator to document observed recall effects and recalled words. Three methods were used to facilitate this experiment: auditory, visual, and hand-writing. Five of the participants were asked to recall freely only 5 Arabic abstract words and 5 concrete words. On the hand, 4 were asked to to recall freely the 10 Arabic abstract words and 10 Arabic concrete words. Results: Descriptive and referential statistics tools were run to analyse the collected date. The computed referential statistics tools indicated generally acceptable values and positive usability of the administered measures in this study. Descriptive statistics results indicated a (100) score for the frequency of zero effect over both abstractness and concreteness effects. Results also indicated a negative effect of list length of Arabic abstract and concrete words where the total number of words was recalled completely in both short and long lists of words. Finally, recency effect approved an advantage over primacy effect with (65%) for the former and only (35%) for the latter. Conclusions: There was neither an advantage for abstract words over concrete ones nor an advantage for concrete words over the abstract ones, it was a rather a zero effect. List length effect didn’t affect memory recall during free recall paradigms. Last but not the least, recency effect is more frequent than primacy effect in free recall paradigms.
    VL  - 4
    IS  - 4
    ER  - 

    Copy | Download

Author Information
  • Department of Linguistics and Translation Studies, College of Languages and Translation, King Saud University, Riyadh, Kingdom of Saudi Arabia

  • Department of Linguistics and Translation Studies, College of Languages and Translation, King Saud University, Riyadh, Kingdom of Saudi Arabia

  • Department of Linguistics, Institute of Social Sciences, Ankara University, S?hhiye, Ankara, Turkey

  • Sections